Introduction

Citizen science can be defined as “public participation in organized research efforts” (), where participants are in most cases involved in data collection or analysis (). Citizen science projects can benefit both researchers and participants, as participants may learn or get excited about science and researchers are given an opportunity to collect data or conduct analyses with the help of many volunteers (; ). Citizen science projects have been developed within a wide range of scientific disciplines, including projects for which participants help with monitoring biodiversity (; ), transcribing old documents (; ), or classifying images ().

While many papers, statements, and guidelines emphasize the importance of providing feedback and acknowledgement to citizen science participants (; ; ), not many papers have examined the extent to which citizen scientists themselves find feedback important. This review gathers available evidence about citizen scientists’ preferences for communication of citizen science project outputs.

Motivations of participants

Because the sustainability of citizen science projects is dependent upon continued public participation, it is important to take motivational factors of participants into account when designing citizen science projects (). Participants often indicate that they are motivated to engage in citizen science because of an interest in a project’s topic (; ; ; ; ; ; ) or in science in general (; ; ; ; ). Participants are also motivated to contribute because they want to learn something new (; ; ; ; ). One of the most often named motivations of participants is to contribute to science or scientific knowledge (; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ). Because contribution to science is such a widely shared motivation for participants, satisfying this motivation is important.

Communication of scientific output

To align with their motivation to contribute to science, participants find it important that the significance of their contributions is clearly communicated (; ; ). Participants’ contribution to research can be recognized by communicating project output during the project as well as at its conclusion. Such communication acknowledges participants and treats them as collaborators with professional scientists, not only as means to an end (; ; ).

In collaborative or co-created projects (), participants may already be engaged in data analysis and thus aware of the project’s results. Therefore, disseminating scientific output may be most important for contributory projects, for which participants may not automatically have access to data and findings.

Vroom’s Expectancy Theory

Participants’ need for communication of project output can be explained by Vroom’s Expectancy Theory (), which has been applied to understand motivational factors for different types of behavior, including engagement in volunteer tourism () and alumni giving to their alma mater (). The theory describes three components of motivation: expectancy, instrumentality, and valence (see Figure 1). Expectancy characterizes a person’s perceived probability that a certain effort will lead to successful performance, and is based on having the right resources, skills, and support to perform the task at hand. Instrumentality construes a person’s perceived probability that successful performance will lead to an outcome, and is concerned with receiving some reward as a result of performance. Valence describes whether a person finds the outcome desirable.

Figure 1 

Vroom’s Expectancy Theory, based on Vroom (). Motivation can be described as being influenced by a combination of three components: Expectancy, instrumentality, and valence.

Vroom’s Expectancy Theory also can be applied to motivational factors for engaging in citizen science. If we define performance as making a contribution to a project, then expectancy relates to a participant’s belief that he can make such a contribution, and is based on self-efficacy for participation, which can be ensured by having enough knowledge to perform the task at hand, having a clear user interface, and providing sufficient information about how to submit a contribution. There can be multiple outcomes of this performance, but one of the most prominent reasons to engage in citizen science has been to contribute to science, and if this is defined as the outcome, then instrumentality means that a participant perceives that his contribution will actually contribute to science. While the desirability of this outcome (valence) can differ by person, we can conclude that the valence of this outcome is generally positive.

Because instrumentality is one of the three key components of motivation, it is important to take it into account when designing a citizen science project. One way to strengthen instrumentality is to communicate scientific output to participants, which enables participants to perceive that their contribution actually leads to a scientific outcome.

Citizen Science principles

The “Ten principles of citizen science” defined by the European Citizen Science Association () also underline this need to communicate project findings and acknowledge participants (). One principle states that project data should be made publicly available and that results should be published in an open access format. Another principle states that citizen scientists need to receive feedback from the project, for example by communicating how participants’ data are used and what the findings are. A third principle states that citizen scientists should be acknowledged in project results and publications.

Three types of scientific output can be recognized in these principles. The first is the data gathered in the project, which should be shared and be accessible. The second is project findings, meaning what project coordinators or researchers have done with the collected data. The third is recognition of project participants in (scientific) publications. Communicating these three types of scientific output can lead to a higher value of Vroom’s component of instrumentality.

This review discusses these three types of output and defines them together as “communication of scientific output.” We use the term “preferences” to indicate what citizen scientists think or find valuable regarding communication of scientific output. Although the importance of communication and feedback to participants is widely accepted among project coordinators, not many academic papers have specifically investigated this topic from the participants’ point of view. Therefore, we provide a systematic literature review of all studies that include participants’ preferences for communication of citizen science project output. This review can serve as a starting point for future research.

Methods

We used the Web of Science database to search for relevant literature on citizen scientists’ views on scientific output. Because citizen science projects are organized in various scientific disciplines, we could not use a domain-specific database. Instead, we followed the approach of West and Pateman () and Kullenberg and Kasperowski () to use the multidisciplinary Web of Science database to search specifically for academic papers (as opposed to Google Scholar, which has more noise). Little research has focused specifically on citizen scientists’ preferences for communication of data, findings, and publications, but we found relevant information in papers studying citizen scientists’ motivations, which generally incorporate citizen scientists’ preferences for different aspects of the project. Therefore, the search term ‘“citizen science” motivat*’ was used to search within “topics.” This approach retrieved 92 papers (see Figure 2 for the literature inclusion process), of which 16 included information on participants’ preferences for communication of citizen science project output. We then examined each paper to see whether it referred to relevant literature beyond the scope of the Web of Science database, resulting in 16 additional papers. See Table 1 for an overview of all included studies.

Figure 2 

Inclusion diagram of studies used in this literature review.

The 32 academic papers that were selected via this process concerned participants’ experiences with engaging in citizen science projects in a wide range of application areas (biodiversity, environmental, astronomy, geography, health). All papers contained results or remarks on participants’ preferences for communication of scientific output. In four papers, relevant information was found in quantitative analyses with either Likert scales or with percentages of participants agreeing with certain statements. In 23 papers, relevant information was found in interviews with participants, open questions in surveys, or in the discussion. In five papers, relevant information was found in both quantitative and qualitative analyses. Many names have been used to refer to those who engage in citizen science projects, for example “citizen scientists”, “volunteers,” or “participants.” For the sake of clarity, we use “participants” for the remainder of this review.

Results

Our findings are structured into three parts concerning participants’ preferences for communication of data, findings, and scientific publications. The first part evaluates participants’ preferences for accessibility of data. The second part discusses communication of citizen science project findings to participants, i.e., what researchers or project leaders have done with the data. The third part consists of an evaluation of participants’ preferences for recognition in scientific publications. Table 1 shows which type of communication every paper touched upon. In total, 12 papers discussed accessibility of data, 20 papers discussed communicating findings, and 11 papers discussed acknowledgment of participants in publications. Papers were generally positive toward communicating scientific output, although 7 papers mentioned practical and ethical issues to take into account when doing so.

Table 1

Overview of studies included in this literature review.

Author(s)YearScientific DisciplineType of communication
Practical and ethical IssuesRelevant information for this review found in quantitative or qualitative analysis?
DataFindingsPublications

Alender, B.2016EnvironmentalXXXQuantitative: Likert scale
Qualitative: Discussion
Baruch, A., May, A. and Yu, D.2016AstronomyXQuantitative: Agreement
Qualitative: Interviews or open questions
Bell, S., Marzano, M., Cent, J., Kobierska, H., Podjed, D., Vandzinskaite, D. et al.2008BiologyXQualitative: Discussion
Bonney, R., Ballard, H., Jordan, R., McCallie, E., Philips, T., Shirk, J. et al.2009aMultipleXQualitative: Discussion
Bonney, R., Cooper, C. B., Dickinson, J., Kelling, S., Phillips, T., Rosenberg, K. V. et al.2009bBiologyXXQuantitative: Observational data Qualitative: Discussion
Bowser, A., Hansen, D., He, Y., Boston, C., Reid, M., Gunnell, L. et al.2013BiologyXXQualitative: Discussion, Interviews or open questions
Brossard, D., Lewenstein, B. and Bonney, R.2005BiologyXQualitative: Discussion
Bruyere, B. and Rappe, S.2007EnvironmentalXQualitative: Discussion
Budhathoki, N. R. and Haythornthwaite, C.2012GeographyXXQuantitative: Likert scale
Carballo-Cárdenas, E. C. and Tobi, H.2016BiologyXXQualitative: Interviews or open questions
Curtis, V.2015BiochemistryXQualitative: Discussion
Dickinson, J. L., Shirk, J., Bonter, D., Bonney, R., Crain, R. L., Martin, J. et al.2012EnvironmentalXXQualitative: Discussion
Domroese, M. C. and Johnson, E. A.2017BiologyXXQualitative: Discussion, Interviews or open questions
Druschke, C. G. and Seltzer, C. E.2012EnvironmentalXQualitative: Discussion
Eveleigh, A., Jennet, C., Blandford, A., Brohan, P. and Cox, A. L.2014EnvironmentalXQualitative: Discussion, Interviews or open questions
Ferster, C. J., Coops, N. C., Harshaw, H. W., Kozak, R. A. and Meitner, M. J.2013EnvironmentalXXQuantitative: Likert scale, Agreement Qualitative: Interviews or open questions
Franzoni, C. and Sauermann, H.2014MultipleXXQualitative: Discussion
Ganzevoort, W. and Born, R. J. G. van den2016BiologyXXQualitative: Interviews or open questions
Ganzevoort, W., Born, R. J. G. van den, Halffman, W. and Turnhout, S.2017BiologyXXXXQuantitative: Agreement Qualitative: Interviews or open questions
Haywood, B. K.2016BiologyXXQualitative: Discussion, Interviews or open questions
Hobbs, S. J. and White, P. C. L.2012BiologyXQuantitative: Agreement
Iacovides, I., Jennet, C. Cornish-Trestrail, C. and Cox, A. L.2013BiochemistryXXQualitative: Interviews or open questions
Jordan, R. C., Gray, S. A., Howe, D. V., Brooks, R. W. and Ehrenfeld, J. G.2011EnvironmentalXQuantitative: Likert scale
Krebs, V.2010HealthXQualitative: Interviews or open questions
Land-Zandstra, A. M., Beusekom, M. M. van, Koppeschaar, C. E. and Broek, J. M. van den2016aHealthXXQualitative: Discussion
Land-Zandstra, A. M., Devilee, J. L. A., Snik, F., Buurmeijer, F. and Broek, J. M. van den2016bEnvironmental, HealthXQuantitative: Agreement
Martin, V., Smith, L., Bowling, A., Christidis, L., Lloyd, D. and Pecl, G.2016BiologyXQualitative: Discussion
Price, C. A. and Lee, H.2013AstronomyXQualitative: Discussion
Rotman, D., Preece, J., Hammock, J., Procita, K., Hansen, D., Parr, C. et al.2012BiologyXXQualitative: Interviews or open questions
Rotman, D., Hammock, J., Preece, J., Hansen, D., Boston, C., Bowser, A. et al.2014MultipleXQualitative: Discussion, Interviews or open questions
See, L., Mooney, P., Foody, G., Bastin, L., Comber, A., Estima, J. et al.2016GeographyXQualitative: Discussion
Tulloch, A. I. T., Possingham, H. P., Joseph, L. N., Szabo, J. and Martin, T. G.2013BiologyXQualitative: Discussion
Total1220117

Accessibility of data

Sharing data with participants

In several of the reviewed studies, participants noted that they would like to get some insights into the data that they collected. According to See et al. (), participants can become motivated because they get something in return such as access to the data for participating in the citizen science project. Likewise, most of the participants in the study of Ferster et al. () on a project that monitored forest fuels agreed with the statement “Data collected by volunteers should be shared with the volunteers who collected them.” Correspondingly, the number of participants in the eBird project on monitoring birds nearly tripled after supplying participants with the ability to track their own observations and compare them with those of others ().

Land-Zandstra et al. () argue that making datasets available for project participants could also foster the learning impact of engaging in citizen science, thereby addressing participants’ motivations to learn something new. In addition, participants can be supplied with data visualization and analysis tools (), which may increase a project’s learning impact (). Providing opportunities for participants in which they can manipulate and study project data may be one of the most educational aspects of citizen science (). Additionally, Druschke and Seltzer () remark that by creating ways for participants to view the data points they have contributed, projects can lead to real engagement among participants in addition to educational impact.

Sharing data with the world

Data collected through citizen science projects also may be shared with other individuals and organizations beyond participants and the project team. Some participants are positive about sharing their collected data not only among themselves, but also with others. Most participants in the study of Ferster et al. () agreed that the data collected by participants should be shared with the general public. Participants in the study of Jordan et al. () on a citizen science project for monitoring invasive plants scored an average 1.7 on a scale from 1 (great extent) to 5 (no extent) on the question “To what extent should scientists share data with the public?” Additionally, participants in the research of Budhathoki and Haythornthwaite () who participated in the OpenStreetMap project on collection of geographic data agreed that their digital map data should be available for free with an average score of 6.45 (out of 7). In the study of Ganzevoort et al. () on several citizen science projects for monitoring biodiversity, 49% of participants felt that the data from their citizen science project are part of the public good. However, 27% felt that the data are owned by the organization running the project, 18% felt that the collected data are private property, and 6% did not know. Participants in the research of Alender () even find public data more important than scientific publications.

Practical and ethical issues

Some authors of the reviewed studies note that sharing data with the public can have some disadvantages. For example, sharing data collected on biodiversity could evoke a rush on rare species, resulting in the disturbance of the natural environment or in creating opportunities for poaching (). Moreover, sharing datasets with the public without professional interpretation may result in data taken out of context, possibly leading to incorrect public understanding of the findings ().

Another issue is whether to make data accessible for commercial organizations, which may use them to make profit. In the study of Ganzevoort et al. (), 37% of participants indicated that their data should not be used for financial gain. Another 26% felt that this issue should be left to the person managing the data, and 16% found sharing data acceptable if the volunteers or the organization are acknowledged. Only 12% were in favor of completely unconditional use of their data (2% had no opinion). In open questions, participants mentioned that project data should be used for the “right” purpose and that volunteer data should not be used by private consultancies (). Most participants in the research of Ferster et al. () were opposed to selling data collected via citizen science projects to private companies. Participants in the research of Budhathoki and Haythornthwaite () indicated that they were hesitant about sharing their data with commercial organizations for free.

Privacy concerns are also present among citizen science participants. In the study of Ferster et al. (), 58% of participants expressed an objection to sharing data being collected on personal private property with the public due to privacy concerns. Participants in the study of Ganzevoort et al. also mentioned that they were concerned about volunteer privacy.

Communication of findings

Some participants mention that they would like to know how their collected data are used in scientific research and what their data are showing (). In the study of Baruch, May, and Yu () on a project aiming at identifying objects and places in satellite images, 28% of the participants noted that they would like to have a follow-up on how the data they collected were used. In the research of Ganzevoort et al. (), 69% of the participants were interested in getting insight into how others use their data. Research performed by Land-Zandstra et al. () indicated that 87% of their participants, who engaged in a citizen science project for measuring aerosols with their smartphone, wanted to know more about what happened with their data. In the study of Alender () on a citizen science project for water quality monitoring, more than 90% of the participants agreed or strongly agreed with the statement “I feel good when data and/or results are shared with me.” Price and Lee () therefore advise to clearly illustrate the participants’ contribution in the overall research project. The following subsections focus on different aspects of communicating findings.

Communicating the value of participants’ contributions

According to Bruyere and Rappe (), people are often motivated to contribute to a project when they feel that their invested time is well spent. Participants feel satisfied when their observations are useful (). Participants often engage in projects with the intention that their contributions directly affect the issue at hand (). Correspondingly, participants want to know in what way their contribution has made an impact (; ; ). Some participants stop contributing because of a perceived lack of value of their data (). Hence, communicating to participants the “usefulness” and value of their data is important (; ). This idea was also expressed by participants in interviews:

“It’s not about spending time or money. It’s more about the constant feedback to the volunteers that what we’re doing is useful and being used.” ()

“If you feel like you’ve done something that they [scientists] couldn’t possibly do because they don’t have enough hours in the day, but you’ve done it, and you’ve helped, then you do really feel part of it. It’s very rewarding.” ()

Learning impact of communicating findings

Another effect of sharing findings with participants may be an increased learning impact (), thereby responding to participants’ motivations to learn something new. In the study of Hobbs and White (), 29% of participants in the Leeds Garden project and 11% of participants in the BirdWatch project stated that the results helped them learn about a bigger picture. Participants in the study of Haywood () on a citizen science project for documenting seabird population health also indicated that they would like to learn about the “big picture” in which their collected data are situated. Haywood () mentions that communicating information on the project as a whole and the position of participants’ data in the project can increase the learning and knowledge gains that participants associate with the project. Citizen science projects aiming for an increased understanding of how science works among participants need to make participants aware of the scientific process and the ways that participants are involved in it ().

Effect of communicating findings on initial and sustained participation

Feedback on a project also can be a prominent motivational factor (). Interviewed participants in the study of Iacovides et al. () on Foldit and Eyewire felt encouraged to contribute to the project when they were given evidence of project progress. Additionally, according to Bell et al. (), communicating information on the project can enhance participants’ satisfaction associated with their participation. Martin et al. () mention that clarifying a project’s findings also can confirm and strengthen the motivation of potential new participants. Correspondingly, demonstrating project findings in a public space may motivate other citizens to participate ().

Demonstrating a project’s findings also may impact sustained participation. Communicating to participants the extent to which their data are used and valued by scientists or policy makers can be considered a key strategy for participant retention (). Additionally, publicizing findings to non-active participants also may have positive effects. In the study of Eveleigh et al. () on the Old Weather project focusing on transcription of old documents such as weather observations, many non-active participants expressed an ongoing interest in the project even when they stop contributing. One participant also mentioned this during interviews:

“I mean, I get all the emails, you know, so I’ll read them and see, you know, what has Old Weather’s community discovered thus far […] the community, as it is, is contributing to science.” ()

When communicating project findings to non-active participants, some may regain their motivation to contribute. Others may remain non-active but still interested in the project, and are possibly great advocates for the project who spread enthusiasm to potential new participants ().

When findings are not clearly communicated, participants can become dissatisfied and demotivated (; ). A lack of clarity on how participants’ data are used is also mentioned by participants as a reason to be less active in the project or even to stop contributing completely (; ). Dissatisfaction when findings are not communicated also was expressed during interviews:

“There was no feedback and it made me feel as though what I was doing wasn’t even for real.” ()

“People won’t come back if there isn’t that loop of credibility and things that they can see that are being accomplished as a result of the data that they are collecting.” ()

Practical and ethical issues

Even though the overall consensus of the literature is that more communication of findings is better, we also specifically searched for possible negative effects of communicating findings to prevent bias in our findings. As a result, we found two cases in which may have dampened motivation. In the study of Domroese and Johnson () on participants’ motivations for engaging in the Great Pollinator Project on monitoring bees, the researchers note two participants who indicated that their motivation changed after they received project results. The authors do not, however, elaborate upon this finding. In the study of Carballo-Cárdenas and Tobi () on a project to monitor lionfish, participants also decided to stop participating because they did not see the importance of monitoring after they learned about initial results. Both cases show that communicating findings can possibly have a negative effect on participation.

Participants’ recognition in scientific publications

Participants also have opinions regarding scientific publications that result from their citizen science projects. In the research of Alender (), slightly more than half of the participants agreed or strongly agreed with the statement “It is important for me that our data are used for scientific publications.” Many participants who strongly agreed that one of their motivations was to contribute to science also agreed that using the data for scientific publications is very important ().

Most importantly, participants would like to be acknowledged in scientific publications, understanding that the data are available only due to their commitment (). Accordingly, in the research of Ganzevoort et al. (), 41% of participants indicated that they like to be cited by name when their data are used. Similarly, approximately 40% of the participants in the study of Alender () found “Name recognition in a scientific publication” meaningful. Participants also reported that they were disappointed upon learning they were not acknowledged in scientific publications (). Likewise, participants in the study of Rotman et al. () expressed the importance of acknowledging them when scientists use their data for publications. Participants also expressed this during interviews:

“If a name ends up in the acknowledgments, the name ends up in a poster, it’s a measurable thing … I can show the family members and make it more of a positive experience.” ()

“Just a name and this X and that Y was contributed by this or that person. Something simple … is like a big thing for a normal person, this kind of thing makes it a very personal thing, and that way we encourage all to do it more …” ()

Participants also have an opinion on the type of journal that a publication in which they are acknowledged gets published in. The participants in the study of Iacovides et al. () point out that they would like to be credited in journals such as Nature. For participants in the study of Rotman et al. (), it did not matter whether the resulting publication was peer-reviewed.

In the study of Alender (), the youngest age group (age 21–29) scored higher on the meaningfulness of name recognition in scientific publications than all other age groups, with an average agreement score of 4.62 out of 5. Alender () notes that younger participants are usually more motivated by advancing their reputation and career than older participants, which may explain this finding. Franzoni and Sauermann () also argue that recognition in publications is most important for those that seek peer recognition.

Tulloch et al. () have also expressed that the contribution of participants should be clearly acknowledged, for example by being recognized in papers or even being named as co-authors in a scientific paper (). Both participants and project coordinators of citizen science projects should be recognized in the acknowledgements section of a resulting paper ().

Practical and ethical issues

Granting all contributing participants authorship in a scientific paper may dilute the value of being an author (), but other ways to recognize the contributions of participants in scientific publications exist. One option is to reward participants who have contributed with a badge and to communicate a list of publications which have used the dataset to participants (). Another method is to use a group pseudonym as (co-)author in order to acknowledge the efforts of the whole group of participants without giving individual credit ().

Discussion

This literature review shows a consensus that communication of scientific output is appreciated by citizen science participants, even though some practical and ethical issues are mentioned. In the papers studying agreement of participants with statements on the importance of communicating scientific output, not all participants (100%) ever agreed. However, it is important to take notice of those participants that do value communication of scientific output. Next we will discuss our findings with regard to Vroom’s Expectancy Theory; consider ethical issues; and provide some advice on how to include communication about scientific output in a project.

Vroom’s Expectancy Theory

In the introduction, we discussed Vroom’s Expectancy Theory to understand factors that motivate citizen science participants. Our review emphasizes the importance of the second component instrumentality, which is based on a participant’s perceived probability that their contribution is actually valuable and will lead to scientific output. Throughout the review, participants have indicated that they like seeing how their contribution has led to an outcome, whether it is a collected dataset, important or interesting findings, or an acknowledgement in a publication. In the case that they do not perceive what happened with their efforts, participants also indicate that their motivation may decrease, leading to a negative effect on sustained participation. This effect is perfectly illustrated by the following quote:

“I’ve done other stuff and you know you don’t get feedback, like “here is what we did with the work you did,” and so you don’t feel like it’s being used well and you don’t feel like you want to continue to contribute.” ()

Hence, by communicating a project’s output, participants can perceive that they contribute to science, which enhances their level of instrumentality and thereby motivation and sustained participation. In addition to the instrumentality component of motivation, project organizers also need to take the first component expectancy into account by making sure that sufficient information is provided and that the interface or activity is clear. Organizers can also have an effect on the valence component, by clearly communicating the importance of the project and its outcomes, for example the importance for monitoring the environment or advancing scientific knowledge. By taking all three components of motivation into account, citizen science projects can strengthen participants’ motivation and thereby participation.

Ethical considerations

The results of our review also suggest some ethical considerations. On the one hand, ethical guidelines agree with participants that communication of output is vital. According to the British Psychological Society (), researchers should consider making the results of their research available to participants. Sales and Folkman () also underline this in their book “Ethics in Research with Human Participants.” They write that participants should be seen as respected partners in the research, and that respect should be communicated toward participants. Additionally, researchers should consider providing information about the nature of the research and available results to participants after their contribution, in order to enhance the educational value of their participation. Moreover, Sales and Folkman () write that researchers should share the collected data whenever possible.

On the other hand, sharing data also may introduce ethical issues in itself. As the 58% of the participants in the study of Ferster et al. () indicated, privacy concerns emerge when data collected on personal private property is shared with the public. The British Psychological Society () also states that privacy of participants should be respected. Sales and Folkman () also note that researchers should not share private information with others and that data should be anonymous without any personal identifiers. Taking privacy into account is especially important because of the new General Data Protection Regulation for EU and EEA citizens ().

Because these guidelines hold for participants in research in general, they also should hold for participants in citizen science, as they should be seen as fellow scientists in the project. To conclude, it is considered ethical to make scientific output as accessible as possible, but personal privacy has to be taken into account at all times.

How to communicate feedback

Because the general consensus of this review is that communicating citizen science output is important for participants, considering how to do this also is important. However, no study in our review has evaluated different ways to communicate project output. Rather, studies have focused on either the method currently used or the possibility of communicating data and output in general. We propose that future research should focus on communication of output to maximize the impact of citizen science projects, including evaluating different data sharing and data comparison options and studying preferred methods of communication.

Based on studies from the gamification field, which is similar to (online) citizen science, one guideline can be to give individual feedback to participants about their own contributions to monitor their own performance (; ). Individual performance feedback can enhance their motivation to continue, because it communicates the performance as a result of a participant’s efforts (). If we translate this back to the citizen science field, an example of feedback in a bird monitoring project could be to generate a visually appealing overview of the different types of birds and their frequencies monitored by an individual participant, preferably with an additional comparison of one’s individual contributions to the findings on a more general (e.g., regional or national) level. Providing individual feedback also can strengthen the instrumentality component of motivation in Vroom’s theory, by clearly showing how one’s individual performance leads to a contribution to science. One direction of future research could be to study the effect of individual versus more general feedback in different types of projects.

Limitations

We have aimed at finding all relevant papers for this review with a systematic approach of searching through Web of Science and literature review sections. Still, some relevant papers may have been left out. We believe, however, that the content of the current review is dense enough to draw conclusions for future research and to make recommendations for project coordinators.

Because not many papers have focused on communication of scientific output, we also have included comments from discussion sections next to information from results and interview sections. We note that the information from discussion sections may be positively biased, because the general consensus in the citizen science field is that communication about scientific output is important.

Conclusion

Guidelines such as the “Ten principles of citizen science” defined by the European Citizen Science Association () emphasize the importance of communicating feedback on the output of a project to participants and recognizing them in results and publications. However, little empirical evidence supports this guideline. From the evidence we could find, we conclude that participants find it important that scientific output of citizen science projects is communicated to them, if ethical issues are taken into account.

The key recommendations for project organizers are displayed in Figure 3. The main recommendation for those designing and executing citizen science projects is to be aware that participants value communication of their collected data, findings of the project, and publications. This preference is supported and explained by Vroom’s Expectancy Theory. Taking this preference into account can pay off, as sharing data and findings may increase the learning impact of a project. It also may enhance participants’ motivation to engage in the project, sustain their participation, and give participants the feeling that their time was well-spent. Moreover, many participants indicate a desire for being acknowledged in publications. Some practical and ethical issues, however, need to be taken into account when developing tools for sharing scientific output, especially in the case of sharing data.

Figure 3 

Key recommendations for citizen science project organizers based on our literature review.

More research on this topic is strongly needed. Most of the papers in our literature review only evaluate communication of scientific output in their discussion section based on miscellaneous data, rather than incorporating the topic into their questionnaires or interviews. Only a few authors have focused on the topic during interviews, with open questions in surveys or with quantitative analyses. No study in our literature review has evaluated how to communicate scientific output to participants. Because the desire of participants for communication of scientific output is evident from the literature and shared among project coordinators, future research should study different methods for communicating citizen science datasets, findings, and publications to maximize the impact of these projects.